Research News

Are they lying? Study reveals what non-verbal clues can and cannot tell you

man with pinocchio nose.

There's no Pinocchio response to lying, says UB communication professor Mark Frank.

By BERT GAMBINI

Published March 26, 2018 This content is archived.

Print
headshot of Mark Frank.
“There is no Pinocchio response. There is nothing like that in a human being. You cannot prove a lie by looking just at behavior. ”
Mark Frank, professor
UB Department of Communication

From dating sites to job interviews to law enforcement, decision-making would be much easier if there was some mannerism that served as a guaranteed indication of a lie.

Unfortunately for the dateless, hiring managers, the police or anyone else who wants to know when someone may or may not be telling the truth, no such signal exists.

However, there often is something in someone’s behavior that can serve as a clue to trigger a search for hard evidence that can settle the uncertainty, according to results of a new study in the Journal of Nonverbal Behavior by Mark Frank, a professor in the UB Department of Communication.

The findings suggest that lie detection is a process often initiated by non-verbal clues, rather than a moment, determined by non-verbal communication.

“There is no Pinocchio response,” says Frank, an expert in non-verbal communication and deception who has worked with many federal law enforcement and intelligence agencies, including the FBI and the CIA. “There is nothing like that in a human being. You cannot prove a lie by looking just at behavior.”

The key, says Frank, is understanding the utility of non-verbal clues, the “hot spots” as he calls them.

“Someone can shrug their shoulders or touch their nose, but there are always other reasons they might be nervous or showing these signs,” he says. “There’s nothing concrete, but these behavioral issues can alert you because while they’re correlated with deception, but they do not prove deception.

“The only way to ever know someone is lying for certain is with unimpeachable, corroborating evidence. And non-verbals can start that process.”

In Frank’s study, led by former UB student Eric Novotny, now a graduate student at Michigan State, and UB students Zachary Carr, Sarah Dietrich, Timothy Shaddock, Megan Cardwell and Andrea Decker, is a response to previous research that suggested non-verbal communication was not useful in detecting lies in the real world.

“Our problem with that research was use of the word, ‘discover,’ which connotes hard evidence at the expense of the process needed to uncover it,” says Frank.

“When we say, ‘The archaeologist discovered bones underground,’ you imagine the archaeologist has bones. But when we say, ‘The archaeologist suspected bones underground,’ you imagine the possibility of bones, but the archaeologist doesn’t have them yet.”

In two separate experiments of roughly 150 participants in each group, Frank and his co-authors asked some people how they “discovered” a lie in their lives and asked others how they “suspected” a lie.

“When we said ‘discover’ we found the same pattern as the other paper, that hard evidence betrayed the lie, with little contribution from non-verbals,” he says. “When we said ‘suspect,’ non-verbal communication was the No. 1 factor that triggered the search for hard evidence.

“It almost flipped the results of the previous research.”

Frank says pop culture can exaggerate the meaning of non-verbal communication, but with lower expectations and an understanding of how to apply non-verbals, people can at least know when perhaps to probe.

“When training law enforcement officers, I tell them that behaviors that don’t fit the situation are not necessarily lies; they’re ‘hot spots’ that can tell you to begin searching for more information.

“Sometimes you’ll find out the person was lying, and other times you won’t, but you’ll never be wrong because you used a non-verbal clue as an alert to look further and simply gather more information, rather than as a determination of truthfulness.”